A deep study between intelligence and crime
The Author is Mr. Rajdeep Singh Chauhan, a student pursuing B.A.LL.B and currently in 8th semester, from LLOYD Law
College, Greater Noida.
We always heard about that a person committed a crime and as
usual, our mentality proved that he is a criminal, he is indeed criminal
because he committed a crime, but we have to think that did he have any other
options so that he could choose that other one and protect himself from the tag
of criminal. According to scriptures, science every coin has two sides,
positive and negative. So every criminal has also some positive elements but we
have to develop those eyes to find positivity from them. We cannot say that
individuals with high or low IQ have a higher chance of engaging in crime. The
criminal intention doesn’t depend on IQ because no person is born criminal.
There are a lot of circumstances behind a person for becoming a criminal. After
committing any criminal activity, every criminal tries to find a way to prove
him innocent. Here we can find out the role of IQ, so every criminal protect
him according to their IQ.
IQ measures the cognitive ability of a person. The low IQ leads to
poor situational decisions and poor analysis of the consequences of certain
behavior. Also, low IQ leads to low cognitive development, low academic
performance, and, thus, to poverty and low socioeconomic status that is S.E.S.
All of these factors could contribute to crime. Those individuals having low IQ
is more easily determined to engage in unlawful behaviors and persuaded to
commit crimes and support consequences.[1] Most of the studies on intelligence and crime demonstrated
that IQ has only an indirect effect on delinquency. Usually, the effect is
mediated by school performance or environment.[2]
Intelligence and Crime
The relationship between intelligence and crime remains a fiercely
debated topic. Despite recent advancements through revised intelligence tests
and sophisticated brain imaging techniques, there remain numerous theoretical
deficiencies regarding the mechanisms underlying the intelligence – crime
relationship. Needless to say, these shortcomings need to be examined more
thoroughly, and new hypotheses must emerge before the role of intelligence in
criminal behavior can be fully explained.
First, the differential detection hypothesis states, in essence,
that criminals with lower intelligence are more likely to be detected by the
police authority for their unlawful activities compared with other criminals
with higher intelligence quotient. In other simple words, we can say,
individuals with higher intelligence may be committing crimes at the same rate
as individuals with lower intelligence, but only the less intelligent ones are
getting caught by the police authority. For that reason, most of the time it is
argued that studies that show a relationship between intelligence and criminal
behavior are invalid because the more intelligent criminals can avoid being
detected by the police.[3]
Classification of Criminals according to their IQ
Many criminologists have written about the relationship between
intelligence and crime, often finding an inverse relationship between these
two. That is, criminologists have found individuals with a below-average IQ are
more likely to commit a criminal offense than higher IQ individuals. The simple
logic is behind this is that the criminals having high IQ use their brains to
find techniques to protect themselves. Some have considered IQ among the most
significant correlates of crime. However, James Oleson’s Criminal Genius sheds
light on the offenses drawn from self-reports and interviews, committed by high
IQ individuals, a group understudied in the field of criminology.
Using a sample was drawn from members of a high IQ society,
college students, and prisoners, Criminal Genius demonstrates that high IQ
offenders defined as individuals with an IQ score of 130 or higher, may commit
more offenses than lower IQ individuals. A novelty of Oleson’s research was the
inclusion of participants from a high IQ society, whose members must score 150
or higher on the IQ test for membership, despite several earlier, failed
efforts to obtain participation from other high IQ societies. Besides, Oleson
analyzed the study’s participant’s involvement in 72 offenses. Nonetheless, a
limitation of the study was the comparability of offenses committed by high IQ
offenders and the low IQ offender’s individuals with IQ scores below the
average IQ of 100 of previous criminological research since the average IQ
score for the control the group was approximately 115, an above-average IQ.[4]
Development of criminal psychology
No person is born criminal, there are some critical circumstances
behind a person who could be addressed as a criminal. If we studied the
development of criminal intention which induced a person to take part in
criminal activities, at the initial level we need to talk about those persons
who have high IQ and those who have low IQ. God created human being and all
humans have equal opportunities to prove themselves. In this world, every
person completes their task according to their capacity, so they use their
brain according to their IQ. But if that task is any criminal activity, then
every criminal tries to get rid of such criminal tag and punishment. In this
context, we will study those criminals who react with their high IQ and low IQ
after committing a crime.
IQ tests generally are reliable enough that most people ages ten
and older have similar IQ scores throughout life.[5] Still, some individuals score very differently when taking
the same test at different times or when taking more than one kind of IQ test
at the same age.[6] For example, many
children in the famous longitudinal Genetic Studies of Genius begun in 1921 by
Lewis Terman showed declines in IQ as they grew up. IQ level cannot
characterize the chances of being criminal. Criminal intention can be
considered are psychology or criminal and its accomplishment can be termed
according to IQ of criminals. We can find the IQ level of any crime according
to their strategy towards crime.
High and low IQ criminals
The majority of studies have found that there are different IQs
between offenders and non-offenders. On average, the IQ for chronic juvenile
offenders is 92, about half a standard deviation below the population means.
For chronic adult offenders, however, the average IQ is 85, 1 standard
deviation below the population mean. A study of Texas inmates who entered the
prison system in 2002 indicated that approximately 23% of the inmates scored
below 80, almost 69% scored between 80 and 109, and only 9.6% scored above 110
IQ level.[7]
This should not be taken as evidence that IQ is unimportant in
delinquency or criminal behavior. When researchers examine self-report data
that are based on measures of relatively serious crime, such as armed robbery,
burglary, or assault, they note substantial IQ differences. Individuals with
relatively lower IQs are more likely to report engaging in these serious
criminal acts. The association between IQ and misbehavior, therefore, depends
on the seriousness of the behavior being analyzed, with the association
becoming stronger as the behavior becomes more serious. Indian legal system
doesn’t provide any provision that the punishment which is given to the
criminals is according to their IQ level; in the context of Indian law, there
should be the same punishment for every criminal for the same crime.
Intelligence and interruptions
We stated earlier that no known social interruptions have
successfully increased IQ scores over the life course. Programs designed to
increase IQ for human being and thus reduce crime and violence is likely to
fail. Even so, this should not be taken as evidence that cognitive
interruptions, in general, are likely to fail. Indeed, quite the opposite is
true. Programs that reduce criminal involvement and violence are more likely to
use principles of cognitive therapy and behavioral modeling.
IQ appears to be immutable after childhood, but individuals, even
those with low IQs can be instructed to recognize criminal thinking patterns
and to alter those patterns. Evidence indicates that IQ is not as important as
the way an individual’s reason, the moral values they hold, or even their level
of impulsivity. Because of this, interruptions that occur early or later in the
life span can be effective in reducing delinquency and crime even if they do
not increase individuals' IQ.
Conclusion
The relationship between intelligence and crime remains a fiercely
debated topic. Despite recent advancements through revised intelligence tests
and sophisticated brain imaging techniques, there remain numerous theoretical
deficiencies regarding the mechanisms underlying the intelligence crime
relationship. Needless to say, these shortcomings need to be examined more
thoroughly, and new hypotheses must emerge before the role of intelligence in
criminal behavior can be fully explained. True understanding may eventually
emerge with the unification of several perspectives from various disciplines;
therefore, anyone cannot forget that intelligence may just be one small piece
of a larger puzzle in which numerous variables taken together can best explain
the cognitive makeup of today’s modern criminal. If a person misuses there high
IQ, it would be problematic for individuals and as well as for the country.
[1] Levine, S. (2011)
Elaboration on the association between IQ and parental SES with the subsequent
crime, Intelligence 50, 1233-1237
[2] Romi,
S.; Marom, D. (2007) Differences in intelligence between no delinquent and
dropout delinquent adolescents, Adolescence, Summer, 2007; 42, 166,325-337
[3] Moffitt,
T., & Silva, P. (1988), IQ and delinquency: A direct test of the
differential detection hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 330–333
[4] James
C. Oleson, CRIMINAL GENIUS: A PORTRAIT OF HIGH-IQ OFFENDERS, (Oakland, CA:
University of California Press, 2016. 300p)
[5] Mackintosh
2011, p. 169 "after the age of 8–10, IQ scores remain relatively stable:
the correlation between IQ scores from age 8 to 18 and IQ at age 40 is over
0.70
[6] Uzieblo
et al. 2012, p. 34 "Despite the increasing disparity between total test
scores across intelligence batteries—as the expanding factor structures cover
an increasing amount of cognitive abilities (Flanagan, et al., 2010)—Floyd et
al. (2008) noted that still 25% of assessed individuals will obtain a 10-point
IQ score difference with another IQ battery. Even though not all studies
indicate significant discrepancies between intelligence batteries at the group
level (e.g., Thompson et al., 1997), the absence of differences at the
individual level cannot be automatically assumed.
[7] Ellis,
L., & Walsh, A. (2003). Crime, delinquency, and intelligence: A review of
the worldwide literature. In H. Nyborg (Ed.), The scientific study of general
intelligence: Tribute to Arthur R. Jensen (pp. 343–366). New York: Pergamon
Press.
Comments
Post a Comment